Operant and classical conditioning are major processes shaping behavioral responses in all animals. Although the understanding of the mechanisms of classical conditioning has expanded significantly, the understanding of the mechanisms of operant conditioning is more limited. Recent developments in Aplysia are helping to narrow the gap in the level of understanding between operant and classical conditioning, and have raised the possibility of studying the neuronal processes underlying the interaction of operant and classical components in a relatively complex learning task. In the present study, we describe a first step toward realizing this goal, by developing a single in vitro preparation in which both operant and classical conditioning can be studied concurrently. The new paradigm reproduced previously published results, even under more conservative and homogenous selection criteria and tonic stimulation regime. Moreover, the observed learning was resistant to delay, shortening, and signaling of reinforcement.
Introduction Ambulatory animals continuously face changing environmental situations.
However, not all events are random occurrences. Some events are direct
consequences either of the behavior of the animal or of some other events in
the environment. If the nonrandom events are significant, animals that can
predict them will have a strong adaptive advantage. Some of the most regular
predictive relationships are inborn (e.g., reflexes), but many others are
learned. Operant or instrumental conditioning is a form of learning in which
an animal learns the predictive relationship between behaviors and the
environment (Thorndike 1911
Laboratory studies of classical conditioning have successfully interrupted
the operant-classical feedback loop such that the behavior of the animal is
irrelevant and the two environmental events (the conditioned stimulus, CS,
which predicts the unconditioned stimulus, US) can be traced from their
sensory afferents to the brain and, finally, to the point where they converge
and the learning occurs (e.g., Walters and
Byrne 1983
The carefully controlled operant and classical conditioning protocols used
in laboratory studies are somewhat artificial learning situations, because the
closed feedback loop between behavioral outputs and sensory inputs in a freely
moving animal inevitably leads to many sensory stimuli eliciting behavioral
responses and many behavioral actions causing the perception of sensory
stimuli, all at or near the same time. One would expect that evolutionary
selection pressures would form around the natural situation in which both
operant and classical predictors play their parts simultaneously, so that this
situation may be more easily learned than in the separate, experimental cases
(i.e., composite conditioning; Brembs
2000
The feeding behavior of Aplysia
(Fig. 1) offers a useful system
in which to investigate classical and operant conditioning. Recently,
substantial progress has been made toward understanding the neurobiology of
operant conditioning of feeding behavior in Aplysia (Nargeot et al.
1997
Given the greater accessibility for neurobiological research, we chose to
work in vitro, with reduced preparations of the Aplysia CNS, similar
to the two previously developed in our laboratory. One in vitro preparation
has been developed to study operant conditioning and another to study
classical conditioning (Nargeot et al.
1997
However, besides the training protocol (operant vs. classical), there is
one major difference between the two preparations. The preparation for
classical conditioning included the cerebral ganglion, because it mediates the
CS pathway (Lechner et al.
2000a Thus, to be able to study the interaction of operant and classical conditioning, we developed a single buccal/cerebral preparation in which classical and operant conditioning experiments can be conducted and the results compared. Moreover, this preparation will allow for the concurrent presentation of classical and operant predictors, and thereby provide a preparation that is suitable for cellular analyses of composite learning. As part of this study, we also developed a computer-assisted neuronal pattern recognition system to identify the BMPs. Most stimulation parameters were entirely computer controlled. The new preparation reproduced the previously published operant learning. Various parameter modifications indicated that the in vitro conditioning was rather robust.
The first step toward developing a preparation in which the interaction of
classical conditioning and operant conditioning can be analyzed was to
determine whether in vitro operant conditioning is expressed in the
preparation originally developed to study classical conditioning
(Mozzachiodi et al. 2003 As part of the study, we also developed a computer program (see Materials and Methods) that allowed for the control of the stimulation schedule and parameters, and to assist in distinguishing between the different types of patterns and therefore eliminate the need for a blind observer. A final aspect of the study was to vary the stimulation parameters to investigate the feasibility of experiments in which operant and classical predictors are combined.
All preparations were treated identically up until the start of the
experiment, where each preparation was randomly assigned to one of six groups
(Fig. 2A,B,C). These groups
were designed as two triplets, the difference between the two being that one
received contingent reinforcement via stimulation of the esophageal nerve and
the other did not (see Materials and Methods for details;
Fig. 2A,B,C). Note that some of
the noncontingent groups received contingent CSs, but never contingent USs.
The groups were all operant in nature and received tonic Bn2,3
stimulation throughout the experiment. This nerve provides afferent input to
the buccal ganglia. Stimulation of Bn2,3 at a constant rate with
weak intensity stimuli increases the likelihood of generating spontaneous BMPs
(Nargeot et al. 1997
Experimental Groups
The respective first groups in each triplet
(Fig. 2A) can be seen as
forming a pair designed to replicate previous studies of in vitro operant
conditioning (Nargeot et al.
1997 The respective second groups (Fig. 2B) were designed to test the effect of a delay and shortening of the reinforcing stimulus (US), as well as the effect of adding a contingent CS without a US. The contingently reinforced group (USdcon, Fig. 2B1) received a contingent US as the "UScon" group. But compared to the UScon group, the US was shortened from 6 to 4 sec and delayed by 2 sec (USdcon, Fig. 2B1). The other group (CS, Fig. 2B2) received only contingent CS presentations and no US presentations. This group was included to control for possible effects of contingent CSs alone (Fig. 2B2). The expected outcome was an elevated number of iBMPs in the USdcon group versus any of the noncontingent groups, and an unaffected number of BMPs in the group that only received a CS, compared to the other two noncontingent groups (i.e., Figs. 2A2, 1C2). Potentially, the USdcon group could have shown a lower number of BMPs than either the UScon (Fig. 2A1) or the CS+USdcon (Fig. 2C1) group. The respective last groups in each triplet (Fig. 2C) were designed to investigate the effect of combining the shortened and delayed US with a contingent CS to "signal" the occurrence of the US (Fig. 2C). Both groups received contingent CS presentations after every iBMP, throughout the experiment. The contingently reinforced group (CS+USdcon) received contingent US presentations after each iBMP/CS combination (Fig. 2C1), whereas the control group (CS+USyoke) received the same sequence of US presentations as the contingently reinforced group, but independent of its behavior (yoked control; Fig. 2C2). In an intact Aplysia, the protocol of CS+USdcon would be analogous to a bite (iBMP) leading to a tactile stimulation of the lips (AT4 stimulation) followed by food (En2 stimulation).
Thus, in the contingently reinforced group
(Fig. 2C1; CS+USdcon), during
training the CS signaled the occurrence of reinforcement (US), whereas in the
yoked control group (Fig. 2C2;
CS+USyoke) it did not. The expected outcome is a higher number of BMPs in the
contingently reinforced as compared to the yoked control. In vertebrates, such
signaling can increase or decrease the amount of operant responding, depending
on the choice of parameters (Williams
1975 BMP Analysis In order to assess the effects of the different treatments on the buccal Central Pattern Generator, three levels of analysis were used. First, we analyzed the total number of BMPs, irrespective of BMP-type. To gather more detailed information, we then analyzed the fraction of BMPs that were ingestion-like in nature (i.e., iBMPs). This measure has the advantage in that it describes the propensity of a preparation to produce iBMPs, irrespective of the total number of patterns produced. Finally, we evaluated the absolute number of iBMPs versus all other BMPs, to gain insight into the absolute changes in the generation of BMPs. A one-way ANOVA (see Materials and Methods) over the total number of BMPs in all six groups did not reveal any significant variations in the total number of BMPs produced, neither in the pretest period immediately preceding the training (SS = 41.5, DF = 5, MS = 8.3, F = 0.48, p = 0.8), nor in the test immediately after the training (SS = 38.1, DF = 5, MS = 7.6, F = 0.38, p = 0.9). Thus, groups did not differ in their propensity to produce BMPs, before or after the training (i.e., treatment did not have any effect on the total number of all BMPs produced by the preparations). Next, the fraction of iBMPs was evaluated. A one-way ANOVA over the six groups in the pretest period immediately preceding the training, was not significant (SS = 0.18; DF = 5; MS = 0.036; F = 0.74; p = 0.6). Thus, the six different groups did not differ significantly in the fraction of iBMPs produced before the training. This result indicates that all preparations had the same propensity to produce ingestion-like BMPs and any difference after training can only be attributed to the parameters of the stimulations during training. All Contingently Reinforced Groups Increased the Propensity to Produce iBMPs A one way ANOVA over the fraction of iBMPs in the six groups in the five minutes immediately following training, was significant (SS = 1.26; DF = 5; MS = 0.25; F = 4.5; p = 0.001). Fisher LSD post-hoc tests reveal that this significance was due to only the contingently reinforced groups differing from all noncontingent groups (Table 1). Thus, none of the different variations in US timing and duration had any effect on the magnitude of learning: contingently reinforced (via stimulation of En2) preparations produced on average a larger fraction of iBMPs than preparations that received either no US at all or noncontingent USs, irrespective of the US parameters (Fig. 3).
Stimulation of the AT4 nerve (such as the CS used here) can also
elicit iBMPs, either after classical conditioning
(Lechner et al. 2000a
The limited number of preparations precludes statistically significant post-hoc differentiation between USdcon, CS+USyoke and CS+USdcon.
We developed a computer-assisted paradigm for in vitro operant and classical conditioning in Aplysia that included the isolated cerebral and buccal ganglia. As a first step we investigated whether the new preparation could exhibit operant conditioning and the robustness of the operant conditioning protocol to parameter variations including the presence of a CS signaling the reinforcer. The new paradigm reproduced previously published results, even under more conservative and homogenous selection criteria and tonic stimulation regime. Moreover, the observed learning was resistant to delay, shortening and signaling of reinforcement. In Vitro Operant Conditioning Is Expressed in the Presence of the Cerebral Ganglion
The previous in vitro analog of operant conditioning consisted of only the
isolated buccal ganglia (Nargeot et al.
1997 In Vitro Operant Conditioning With the Cerebral Ganglion is a Robust Phenomenon We found that shortening and delaying the reinforcement by 2 sec did not disrupt the operant learning. We further found that adding a 2-sec CS between the ingestion-like BMPs and the reinforcement (US) also neither increased nor decreased the operant behavior.
Interestingly, delayed reinforcement is known from vertebrates to generally
decrease the rate at which the operant behavior controlling the reinforcement
is produced (e.g., Williams et al.
1990
Importantly, the presentation of a sensory signal (or operant CS; the 2-sec
AT4 stimulation) of reinforcement in the delay after a BMP and
before reinforcement does not disrupt or enhance the production of
ingestion-like BMPs, compared to the situation in which the US is merely
delayed. This paradigm would be analogous to a behavior controlling both a
predictive neutral stimulus (the CS) and a biologically relevant one (the US)
at the same time. Returning to the example of a frog trying to capture a bee,
extending the tongue would lead to a sting (US) by the striped bee (CS). In an
intact Aplysia, the protocol would be analogous to a bite
(ingestion-like BMP) leading to a tactile stimulation of the lips
(AT4 stimulation) followed by food (En2 stimulation). It
is easy to assume that the tactile lip stimulus may be interpreted as the food
item moving, caused by the biting and swallowing movements. In both cases, the
operant (the tongue extension or the bite) and the classical (the stripes of
the bee or the lip stimulation) predictors can be perceived as competitors in
the animal's search for a predictor of the reinforcer
(Rescorla 1994 Thus, the operant effect described by Nargeot and colleagues is a robust, reproducible case of operant conditioning with the potential to study an even wider variety of behavior-CS-US relationships than space permits to present here. Differences Between Previous Work
One of the results in Nargeot et al.
(1997 Outlook
In the future, this in vitro operant/classical conditioning paradigm can be
employed to examine such long-standing questions as whether there are any
operant components even in purely classical conditioning (e.g.,
Gormezano and Tait 1976
General Methods
Aplysia californica (80-350 g) were obtained from Alacrity Marine
Biological Specimens and Marinus and housed individually in perforated plastic
cages, floating in aerated seawater tanks at 15°C. Animals were fed Dissection
Prior to dissection, the motivational state of all animals was enhanced by
first feeding them a small piece of dried seaweed ( Extracellular Nerve Recordings
Previous in vivo recordings indicate that bursts of large-unit activity in
nerves I2n,Rn1 and Bn2,1 are associated with
the protraction, closure, and retraction, respectively, of the
radula/odontophore during feeding (Morton
and Chiel 1993b Extracellular Nerve Stimulation
Similar to our previous studies (Nargeot et al.
1997
Electrical stimulation of AT4 (2 sec, 5 Hz, 0.5-msec pulses) was
used to mimic the CS that was used in classical conditioning in vivo (Lechner
et al.
2000a
Following Nargeot et al.
(1997 Pulses for extracellular nerve stimulation were generated by a digital pulse generator (Pulsemaster A300, WPI) and applied, via a stimulus isolator (A360; WPI, Sarasota, FL), to bipolar silver electrodes that were placed on nerves Bn2,3, AT4, and En2 and isolated from the bath with Vaseline. Once the extracellular electrodes were in place, the high divalent ASW was exchanged for normal ASW. Preparations were washed with 50 ml ASW and then single stimulations were applied to each of the three nerves to verify electrode connectivity. Pilot studies showed that due to the high incidence of BMPs immediately after the tonic stimulation of Bn2,3 was switched on, it was impossible to determine the appropriate sub-threshold AT4 intensity during Bn2,3 stimulation. Therefore, the intensity was empirically set to 3 V for all operant preparations, an intensity that on its own did not increase the number of BMPs in the pilot studies. Classifications of BMPs
The feeding CPG expresses BMPs, which can be associated with ingestion or
rejection of food (Morton and Chiel
1993a
As in previous studies (Morton and Chiel
1993a In the present study, only patterns that consisted of activity in all three buccal nerves clustered in a complete protraction/retraction cycle were classified as BMPs. Patterns consisting of bursts of activity in only one or two of the three nerves were classified as incomplete patterns and were not included in the study. Computer-Assisted BMP Recognition
The custom-written software provided computer-assisted pattern recognition
(i.e., the computer attempted an online classification and suggested a pattern
type at the end of each BMP). The software was written on a MS Windows based
PC using C++ and the provided software development kit for the PCI 9112
converter card. The acquisition rate was limited by processor speed, in our
case to Procedures for In Vitro Training
The procedures were based on the in vitro operant conditioning experiment
developed by Nargeot et al.
(1997 Animals were divided randomly in six groups. Each group received tonic stimulation of Bn2,3, which began after the 30-min rest period and continued uninterrupted until the experiment ended. The groups differed from each other by the application regime of CS and US applications.
The first two groups were designed to replicate previous findings
(Nargeot et al. 1997 The third group was designed to test for the effect of a delay and shortening of the US (USdcon). This group received a contingent 4-sec US with a 2-sec delay after each ingestion-like BMP produced during training. The fourth group was designed to test the effect of introducing contingent CSs after each iBMP without a US. This group (CS) received contingent 2-sec AT4 stimulations (operant CSs) immediately after each ingestion-like BMP throughout the experiment and no USs during the training period. The last two groups were designed to test the effects of introducing a signal of the delayed US. Both groups received contingent 2-sec AT4 stimulations (operant CSs) immediately after each ingestion-like BMP throughout the experiment, starting after the 30-min rest period. During training, the CS+USdcon group received contingent reinforcement (operant 4-sec USs) immediately upon cessation of the operant CS after each ingestion-like BMP. Thus, each ingestion-like BMP in this group was followed first by a CS and then by a US; both stimulations together yielded a total of 6 sec of stimulation after each ingestion-like BMP (the US in Nargeot and colleagues original experiment had been 6 sec as well). The CS+USyoke group received the same sequence of 4-sec En2 stimulations during the training period as the CS+USdcon group, but uncorrelated with either generated BMPs or received CSs (yoked control). Preparations that did not produce at least one ingestion-like BMP during training and at least three ingestion-like BMPs in the entire experiment were discarded. Statistics One-way or multifactor Analyses of Variance (ANOVAs) were carried out to estimate the significance of within- and between-group differences. Fisher LSD Post-hoc tests were used to detect the significant contributions to the variance in the data.
We thank R. Mozzachiodi for helpful comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript. Supported by an Emmy-Noether fellowship (B.B.) and NIH Research Grant R01 MH58423 (J.H.B.). The publication costs of this article were defrayed in part by payment of page charges. This article must therefore be hereby marked "advertisement" in accordance with 18 USC section 1734 solely to indicate this fact.
Article published online ahead of print. Article and publication date are at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/content/full/11/4/412.
1 Present address: Institute for Neurobiology, Free University Berlin,
Königin-Luise-Straße 28/30, 14195 Berlin, Germany. 2 E-MAIL bjoern@brembs.net;FAX 49 308 385 5455.
Anderson, J.A. 1967. Patterns of response of neurons in the cerebral ganglion of Aplysia californica. Exp. Neurol. 19:65-77.[Medline] Antonov, I., Antonova, I., Kandel, E.R., and Hawkins, R.D.2003. Activity-dependent presynaptic facilitation and hebbian ltp are both required and interact during classical conditioning in Aplysia. Neuron37:135-147.[Medline]
Bao, J.X., Kandel, E.R., and Hawkins, R.D. 1998.
Involvement of presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms in a cellular analog of
classical conditioning at Aplysia sensory-motor neuron synapses in
isolated cell culture. J. Neurosci.
18:458
-466. Brembs, B. 2000. An analysis of associative conditioning in Drosophila at the flight simulator. Ph.D. thesis, University of Würzburg, Germany. http://opus.bibliothek.uni-wuerzburg.de/opus/volltexte/2002/103/pdf/diss.pdf
Brembs, B. and Heisenberg, M. 2000. The operant and
the classical in conditioned orientation in Drosophila melanogaster
at the flight simulator. Learn. Mem.
7: 104-115.
Brembs, B., Lorenzetti, F.D., Reyes, F.D., Baxter, D.A., and Byrne,
J.H. 2002. Operant reward learning in Aplysia: Neuronal
correlates and mechanisms. Science
296:1706
-1709.
Byrne, J.H., Castellucci, V.F., and Kandel, E.R. 1978.
Contribution of individual mechanoreceptor sensory neurons to defensive
gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. J.
Neurophysiol. 41:418
-431. Colwill, R., Goodrum, K., and Martin, A. 1997. Pavlovian appetitive discriminative conditioning in Aplysia californica. Anim Learn. Behav. 25:268 -276. Corbit, L.H., Muir, J.L., and Balleine, B.W. 2003. Lesions of mediodorsal thalamus and anterior thalamic nuclei produce dissociable effects on instrumental conditioning in rats. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18:1286 -1294.[CrossRef][Medline] Cropper, E.C., Kupfermann, I., and Weiss, K.R. 1990. Differential firing patterns of the peptide-containing cholinergic motor neurons b15 and b16 during feeding behavior in Aplysia. Brain Res. 522:176 -179.[CrossRef][Medline]
Crow, T. and Tian, L.M. 2003. Neural correlates of
Pavlovian conditioning in components of the neural network supporting ciliary
locomotion in Hermissenda. Learn. Mem.
10:209
-216.
Davis, M., Walker, D.L., and Myers, K.M. 2003. Role of
the amygdala in fear extinction measured with potentiated startle.
Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci.
985:218
-232. Epstein, H.T., Child, F.M., Kuzirian, A.M., and Alkon, D.L.2003 . Time windows for effects of protein synthesis inhibitors on Pavlovian conditioning in Hermissenda: Behavioral aspects. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 79:127 -131.[CrossRef][Medline] Flynn, M., Cai, Y., Baxter, D.A., and Crow, T. 2003. A computational study of the role of spike broadening in synaptic facilitation of Hermissenda. J. Comput. Neurosci. 15: 29-41.[CrossRef][Medline] Fredman, S.M. and Jahan-Parwar, B. 1980. Processing of chemosensory and mechanosensory information in identifiable Aplysia neurons. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A 66: 25-34.[CrossRef] Gormezano, I. and Tait, R.W. 1976. The Pavlovian analysis of instrumental conditioning. Pavlov. J. Biol. Sci. 11:37 -55.[Medline] Hammerl, M. 1993. Blocking observed in human instrumental conditioning. Learn. Motiv. 24: 73-87.[CrossRef] Hawkins, R.D., Greene, W., and Kandel, E.R. 1998. Classical conditioning, differential conditioning, and second-order conditioning of the Aplysia gill-withdrawal reflex in a simplified mantle organ preparation. Behav. Neurosci. 112:636 -645.[CrossRef][Medline]
Heisenberg, M., Wolf, R., and Brembs, B. 2001.
Flexibility in a single behavioral variable of Drosophila.
Learn. Mem. 8:1
-10. Holland, P.C. and Gallagher, M. 2003. Double dissociation of the effects of lesions of basolateral and central amygdala on conditioned stimulus-potentiated feeding and Pavlovian-instrumental transfer. Eur. J. Neurosci. 17:1680 -1694.[Medline]
Hurwitz, I., Neustadter, D., Morton, D.W., Chiel, H.J., and
Susswein, A.J. 1996. Activity patterns of the b31/b32 pattern
initiators innervating the i2 muscle of the buccal mass during normal feeding
movements in Aplysia californica. J.
Neurophysiol. 75:1309
-1326.
Hurwitz, I., Kupfermann, I., and Weiss, K.R. 2003.
Fast synaptic connections from cbis to pattern-generating neurons in
Aplysia: Initiation and modification of motor programs. J.
Neurophysiol. 89:2120
-2136. Jahan-Parwar, B. and Fredman, S.M. 1976. Cerebral ganglion of Aplysia: Cellular organization and origin of nerves. Comp. Biochem. Physiol. A 54:347 -357.[Medline]
Jing, J. and Weiss, K.R. 2001. Neural mechanisms of
motor program switching in Aplysia. J.
Neurosci. 21:7349
-7362.
____. 2002. Interneuronal basis of the generation of
related but distinct motor programs in Aplysia: Implications for
current neuronal models of vertebrate intralimb coordination. J.
Neurosci. 22:6228
-6238.
Kabotyanski, E.A., Baxter, D.A., Cushman, S.J., and Byrne, J.H.2000
. Modulation of fictive feeding by dopamine and serotonin in
Aplysia. J. Neurophysiol.
83:374
-392.
Katzoff, A., Ben-Gedalya, T., and Susswein, A.J. 2002.
Nitric oxide is necessary for multiple memory processes after learning that a
food is inedible in Aplysia. J. Neurosci.
22:9581
-9594. Kim, J.J., Krupa, D.J., and Thompson, R.F. 1998. Inhibitory cerebello-olivary projections and blocking effect in classical conditioning. Science 279:570 -573. Konorski, J. and Miller, S. 1937a. On two types of conditioned reflex. J. Gen. Psychol. 16:264 -272. ____. 1937b. Further remarks on two types of conditioned reflex. J. Gen. Psychol. 17:405 -407. Kuslansky, B., Weiss, K.R., and Kupfermann, I. 1978. A neural pathway mediating satiation of feeding behavior in Aplysia. Behav. Biol. 23:230 -237.[Medline] Kuslansky, B., Weiss, K.R., and Kupfermann, I. 1987. Mechanisms underlying satiation of feeding behavior of the mollusc Aplysia. Behav. Neural Biol. 48:278 -303.[Medline]
Lechner, H.A., Baxter, D.A., and Byrne, J.H. 2000a.
Classical conditioning of feeding in Aplysia: II. Neurophysiological
correlates. J. Neurosci.
20:3377
-3386.
____. 2000b. Classical conditioning of feeding in
Aplysia: I. Behavioral analysis. J. Neurosci.
20:3369
-3376.
Lloyd, P.E., Kupfermann, I., and Weiss, K.R. 1988.
Central peptidergic neurons regulate gut motility in Aplysia.
J. Neurophysiol. 59:1613
-1626. Medina, J.F., Christopher Repa, J., Mauk, M.D., and LeDoux, J.E.2002 . Parallels between cerebellum- and amygdala-dependent conditioning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3: 122-131.[CrossRef][Medline] Morton, D.W. and Chiel, H.J. 1993a. In vivo buccal nerve activity that distinguishes ingestion from rejection can be used to predict behavioral transitions in Aplysia. J. Comp. Physiol. A 172:17 -32.[Medline] ____. 1993b. The timing of activity in motor neurons that produce radula movements distinguishes ingestion from rejection in Aplysia. J. Comp. Physiol. A 173:519 -536.[Medline]
Mozzachiodi, R., Lechner, H., Baxter, D., and Byrne, J.2003
. An in vitro analogue of classical conditioning of feeding
behavior in Aplysia. Learn. Mem.
10:478
-494. Nader, K. 2003. Memory traces unbound. Trends Neurosci. 26:65 -72.[CrossRef][Medline]
Nargeot, R., Baxter, D.A., and Byrne, J.H. 1997.
Contingent-dependent enhancement of rhythmic motor patterns: An in vitro
analog of operant conditioning. J. Neurosci.
17:8093
-8105.
____. 1999a. In vitro analog of operant conditioning
in Aplysia. I. Contingent reinforcement modifies the functional
dynamics of an identified neuron. J. Neurosci.
19:2247
-2260.
____. 1999b. In vitro analog of operant conditioning
in Aplysia. II. Modifications of the functional dynamics of an
identified neuron contribute to motor pattern selection. J.
Neurosci. 19:2261
-2272.
____. 1999c. Dopaminergic synapses mediate neuronal
changes in an analogue of operant conditioning. J.
Neurophysiol. 81:1983
-1987.
Paschall, G.Y. and Davis, M. 2002. Second-order
olfactory-mediated fear-potentiated startle. Learn.
Mem. 9:395
-401. Pavlov, I.P. 1927. Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.
Perrins, R. and Weiss, K.R. 1996. A cerebral central
pattern generator in Aplysia and its connections with buccal feeding
circuitry. J. Neurosci.
16:7030
-7045. Phillips, G.D., Setzu, E., Vugler, A., and Hitchcott, P.K.2003 . Immunohistochemical assessment of mesotelencephalic dopamine activity during the acquisition and expression of Pavlovian versus instrumental behaviours. Neuroscience 117:755 -767.[CrossRef][Medline] Reed, P. 1992a. Effect of a signaled delay between an action and outcome on human judgment of causality. Q.J. Exp. Psychol. B 44B:81 -100. ____. 1992b. Signaled delay of reward—overshadowing versus sign-tracking explanations. Learn. Motiv. 23:27 -42. ____. 1996. Visual reinforcement signals interfere with the effects of reinforcer magnitude manipulations. Learn. Motiv. 27:464 -475.[CrossRef][Medline] ____. 1999. Role of a stimulus filling an action-outcome delay in human judgments of causal effectiveness. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 25: 92-102.[CrossRef][Medline] ____. 2003. The effect of signaled reinforcement on rats' fixed-interval responding. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 79:367 -382.[Medline] Rescorla, R.A. 1990a. Evidence for an association between the discriminative stimulus and the response-outcome association in instrumental learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 16:326 -334.[CrossRef][Medline] ____. 1990b. The role of information about the response-outcome relation in instrumental discrimination learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 16:262 -270.[CrossRef][Medline] ____. 1994. Control of instrumental performance by Pavlovian and instrumental stimuli. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Process. 20:44 -50.[CrossRef][Medline] Rescorla, R.A. and Holland, P.C. 1982. Behavioral studies of associative learning in animals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 33:265 -308.[CrossRef] Rescorla, R.A. and Solomon, R.L. 1967. Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychol. Rev. 74:151 -182.[Medline]
Ressler, K.J., Paschall, G., Zhou, X.L., and Davis, M.2002
. Regulation of synaptic plasticity genes during
consolidation of fear conditioning. J. Neurosci.
22:7892
-7902.
Rosen, S.C., Weiss, K.R., and Kupfermann, I. 1979.
Response properties and synaptic connections of mechanoafferent neurons in
cerebral ganglion of Aplysia. J.
Neurophysiol. 42:954
-974. Rosen, S.C., Teyke, T., Miller, M.W., Weiss, K.R., and Kupfermann, I. 1991. Identification and characterization of cerebral-to-buccal interneurons implicated in the control of motor programs associated with feeding in Aplysia. J. Neurosci. 11:3630 -3655.[Abstract] Schafe, G.E., Nader, K., Blair, H.T., and LeDoux, J.E.2001 . Memory consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning: A cellular and molecular perspective. Trends Neurosci. 24:540 -546.[CrossRef][Medline] Schwarz, M. and Susswein, A.J. 1986. Identification of the neural pathway for reinforcement of feeding when Aplysia learn that food is inedible. J. Neurosci. 6:1528 -1536.[Abstract] Skinner, B.F. 1935. Two types of conditioned reflex and a pseudo type. J. Gen. Psychol. 12: 66-77. ____. 1937. Two types of conditioned reflex: A reply to Konorski and Miller. J. Gen. Psychol. 16:272 -279. ____. 1938. The behavior of organisms. Appleton, New York. Susswein, A.J. and Schwarz, M. 1983. A learned change of response to inedible food in Aplysia. Behav. Neural Biol. 39:1 -6.[Medline] Susswein, A.J., Gev, S., Feldman, E., and Markovich, S.1983 . Activity patterns and time budgeting of Aplysia fasciata under field and laboratory conditions. Behav. Neural Biol. 39:203 -220.[Medline] Susswein, A.J., Weiss, K.R., and Kupfermann, I. 1984. Internal stimuli enhance feeding behavior in the mollusc Aplysia. Behav. Neural Biol. 41:90 -95.[Medline] Susswein, A.J., Schwarz, M., and Feldman, E. 1986. Learned changes of feeding behavior in Aplysia in response to edible and inedible foods. J. Neurosci. 6:1513 -1527.[Abstract] Thorndike, E.L. 1911. Animal intelligence. Macmillan, New York. Trapold, M.A. and Overmier, J.B. 1972. The second learning process in instrumental conditioning. In Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (eds. A.H. Black and W.F. Prokasy), pp. 427-452. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York. Trapold, M.A. and Winokur, S. 1967. Transfer from classical conditioning and extinction to acquisition, extinction, and stimulus generalization of a positively reinforced instrumental response. J. Exp. Psychol. 73:517 -525.[Medline] Trapold, M.A., Lawton, G.W., Dick, R.A., and Gross, D.M.1968 . Transfer of training from differential classical to differential instrumental conditioning. J. Exp. Psychol. 76:568 -573.[Medline] Walters, E.T. and Byrne, J.H. 1983. Associative conditioning of single sensory neurons suggests a cellular mechanism for learning. Science 219:405 -408.[Medline] Williams, B.A. 1975. The blocking of reinforcement control. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 24:215 -225. ____. 1989. Signal duration and suppression of operant responding by free reinforcement. Learn. Motiv. 20:335 -357. ____. 1999. Associative competition in operant conditioning: Blocking the response-reinforcer association. Psych. Bull. Rev. 6:618 -623. Williams, B.A. and Heyneman, N. 1982. Multiple determinants of blocking effects on operant behavior. Anim. Learn. Behav. 10:72 -76. Williams, B.A., Preston, R.A., and DeKervor, D.E.1990 . Blocking of the response-reinforcer association additional evidence. Learn. Motiv. 21:379 -398. Xin, Y., Weiss, K.R., and Kupfermann, I. 1995. Distribution in the central nervous system of Aplysia of afferent fibers arising from cell bodies located in the periphery. J. Comp. Neurol. 359:627 -643.[Medline] |